Objective: To explore the mediational effects of prejudice on the relationship between negative stereotypes and social distance (discrimination) in a sample of Veterans Administration health care providers.
Methods: Data for this study were collected between August 2011 and April 2012 as part of a larger study examining provider attitudes and clinical expectations toward 2 hypothetical vignette patients: 1 with schizophrenia and 1 without schizophrenia. Survey responses from health care providers were gathered using 3 well-recognized measures: the 9-item Semantic Differential Scale, 9-item Attribution Questionnaire, and Social Distance Scale. A path model was tested using Mplus version 6 to investigate whether prejudice mediates the relation between provider stereotyping and social distance.
Results: A total of 351 health care providers responded to the survey. The results indicate that there was a significant positive correlation between provider stereotypes and prejudice (β = 0.298, P < .0001), and prejudice significantly predicted social distance (β = 0.190, P = .002). The indirect effect of stereotypes on social distance, tested using bootstrapped standard errors, was also statistically significant (β = 0.167, P = .007).
Conclusions: Findings from this study confirm the important role of affective reactions (prejudice) in generating discriminatory behavior (social distancing) among health care providers. The findings will also help future researchers identify potential targets for interventions to decrease stigma among health care providers.
Relationship Between Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Social Distancing in a Sample of Health Care Providers
ABSTRACT
Objective: To explore the mediational effects of prejudice on the relationship between negative stereotypes and social distance (discrimination) in a sample of Veterans Administration health care providers.
Methods: Data for this study were collected between August 2011 and April 2012 as part of a larger study examining provider attitudes and clinical expectations toward 2 hypothetical vignette patients: 1 with schizophrenia and 1 without schizophrenia. Survey responses from health care providers were gathered using 3 well-recognized measures: the 9-item Semantic Differential Scale, 9-item Attribution Questionnaire, and Social Distance Scale. A path model was tested using Mplus version 6 to investigate whether prejudice mediates the relation between provider stereotyping and social distance.
Results: A total of 351 health care providers responded to the survey. The results indicate that there was a significant positive correlation between provider stereotypes and prejudice (β = 0.298, P < .0001), and prejudice significantly predicted social distance (β = 0.190, P = .002). The indirect effect of stereotypes on social distance, tested using bootstrapped standard errors, was also statistically significant (β = 0.167, P = .007).
Conclusions: Findings from this study confirm the important role of affective reactions (prejudice) in generating discriminatory behavior (social distancing) among health care providers. The findings will also help future researchers identify potential targets for interventions to decrease stigma among health care providers.
Prim Care Companion CNS Disord 2018;20(3):17m02264
To cite: Chekuri L, Mittal D, Ounpraseuth S. Relationship between stereotypes, prejudice, and social distancing in a sample of health care providers. Prim Care Companion CNS Disord. 2018;20(3):17m02264.
To share: https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.17m02264
© Copyright 2018 Physicians Postgraduate Press, Inc.
aDepartment of Applied Gerontology, University of North Texas, Denton, Texas
bDepartment of Psychiatry, Mercy Hospital, Joplin, Missouri
cVA Center for Mental Healthcare and Outcomes Research, Central Arkansas Veterans Healthcare System (CAVHS), North Little Rock, Arkansas
dPsychiatric Research Institute, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, Arkansas
eDepartment of Biostatistics, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, Arkansas
*Corresponding author: Lakshminarayana Chekuri, MD, MPH, Mercy Behavioral Health, 5615 W 32nd St, Joplin, MO 64804 ([email protected]).
Stigma is a composite of negative stereotypes and prejudice that results in discriminatory behavior.1 Stereotypes are negative beliefs about members of a group. Such cognitive knowledge structures often generate impressions and expectations about individuals who belong to a particular group. Prejudicial attitudes, in contrast, are affective reactions toward members of a group; they are implicated in the endorsement of negative stereotypes, which can further lead to discrimination.1,2 This relationship between stigmatizing stereotypes and prejudicial attitudes and their influence on discriminatory behavior has been well established in studies1,2 focused on the general public. While negative attitudes and beliefs endorsed by the general public toward individuals with serious mental illness (SMI) are known to affect their self-esteem, increase social distancing, and decrease help seeking,3 health care providers’ negative attitudes and stereotypes may affect quality of care delivered to these individuals.4 Ironically, several studies5-10 have found attitudes and beliefs held by health care providers to be either comparable to or worse than those of the general public. Health care providers in those studies5-10 endorsed involuntary treatment of individuals with SMI, restriction of their civil rights, and negative beliefs about whether individuals with SMI should be informed of their diagnosis or given treatment choices.
Consequences of stigmatizing behaviors toward individuals with SMI are enormous and affect not only the individual who is being stigmatized but also the overall health care delivery system. Stigma toward individuals with SMI is identified as a barrier to both health care seeking and treatment engagement.11 Stigma is also implicated in the clinical decision-making process of health care providers.12 Stigmatizing attitudes may result in social distancing from individuals with SMI.13 Findings from several experiments conducted by Jussim and colleagues2 suggest that controlling for prejudice eliminates bias and thus could be used to decrease the effects of stigma to some extent. These experiments also suggest that some individuals who are aware of their negative stereotypes about members of a particular group may or may not agree with such stereotypes and hence are able to control their discriminatory behavior. While these experiments were conducted in a sample of college students,2 it is not fully understood if such conclusions are generalizable to health care providers.
Limited information is available in the literature on the interplay between stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination among health care providers. Health care providers are a distinct group that has a superior understanding of the biological basis of health conditions including SMI. Given their understanding of the biological nature of SMI, one would expect health care providers to hold fewer stereotypical beliefs compared to the general public. However, it is interesting to note that studies8 have reported prevalence rates of stigmatizing attitudes among health care providers comparable to the general population. These studies8 also reported that diagnostic labels such as schizophrenia continue to evoke negative affective reactions among health care providers. Sullivan and colleagues14 compared responses of clinicians to a vignette patient with schizophrenia with responses to a patient without schizophrenia. Clinicians expected the vignette patient with schizophrenia to be less adherent to treatment, less able to read and understand educational materials, and less capable of managing health and personal affairs. Clinicians who responded in this study were also less likely to refer the vignette patient with schizophrenia to a weight-reduction program compared to the vignette patient without schizophrenia.14 Likewise, Corrigan and colleagues12 examined the relationship between stigmatizing attitudes and physicians’ health care decisions and found that physicians who endorsed stigmatizing characteristics were more likely to believe the patient would not adhere to treatment. Such physicians were also less likely to refer the patient to a specialist or refill prescriptions.12 Implications of such negative expectations are paramount and would directly affect the overall quality of care received by patients with SMI. While these findings establish the effect of stigmatizing stereotypes and prejudice on the clinical decision-making process of health care providers, it is unclear to what extent prejudice plays a role in mediating the effects of stigmatizing stereotypes on health care providers’ tendency to engage in behaviors such as social distancing from persons with SMI.
On the basis of findings in the general public, we hypothesized that the relationship between negative stereotypes and social distance (discrimination) will be mediated by prejudice and that the combined effects of stigmatizing stereotypes and prejudice would result in a greater desire for social distance. For these analyses, we utilized a previously used dataset to examine the relationship between stigmatizing attitudes and health care decisions among providers.12,15,16
- Significant disparities in quality of health care delivered to individuals with serious mental illness (SMI) have been reported, possibly due to health care providers’ negative attitudes and low expectations for treatment in these individuals.
- The study findings highlight the mediational role of prejudice on the relationship between negative stereotypes and discrimination toward individuals with SMI.
- Health care providers should be aware of their negative stereotypes and automatic affective responses when offering treatment to individuals with SMI.
METHODS
Data Collection
Data for this study were collected as part of a larger study examining provider attitudes and clinical expectations toward 2 hypothetical vignette patients: 1 with schizophrenia and 1 without schizophrenia.14,16 Details of the original study16 are described elsewhere. Briefly, data were gathered between August 2011 and April 2012 using a survey of 351 health care providers (67 mental health nurses, 91 primary care nurses, 55 physicians, 62 psychiatrists, and 76 psychologists) from 5 Veterans Affairs (VA) health care settings in the southeast and south central areas of the United States. The study was approved by the VA Central Institutional Review Board.
Measures
Negative stereotypes held by health care providers surveyed in this study were assessed using a 9-item Semantic Differential Scale derived from the Characteristics Scale.17 Each provider rated the qualities (such as valuable and worthless, safe and dangerous, strong and weak, predictable and unpredictable) of the hypothetical patient from the vignette using a 7-point Likert scale. The responses on the 9 items were summed, with the total score representing the severity of provider stereotyping. Higher scores on the scale denote more negative attitudes toward the patient. The α reliability coefficient (Cronbach α) for this scale was 0.83.
Prejudicial attitudes held by health care providers surveyed in this study were assessed using the 9-item Attribution Questionnaire (AQ-9).18 The AQ-9 assesses a subject’s emotional responses such as anger, pity, and fear on a 9-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 9 = very much). Exploratory factor analysis confirmed an adequate fit for 6 items. The 6 item responses were summed for the analysis, with higher scores denoting more negative attributions about the patient. The α reliability coefficient (Cronbach α) for this scale was 0.76.
Discriminatory behavioral intentions of health care providers surveyed for this study were assessed using items included in the Social Distance Scale, which comprises 5 items from the National Opinion Research Center General Social Survey.19 Providers were asked to rate each item on a 4-point Likert scale. Item scores were summed, with higher scores denoting a greater desire for social distance from the hypothetical vignette patient. The α reliability coefficient (Cronbach α) for this scale was 0.86.
Data Analysis
To investigate whether prejudice mediates the relation between provider stereotyping and social distance, a path model was tested using Mplus version 6.20 We controlled for several key demographics in the path model, including provider, sex, age, race (white vs non-white), personal and professional provider contact with mental illness, provider specialty, and number of years in clinical practice.
RESULTS
A total of 351 health care providers responded to the randomly distributed surveys. A total of 192 health care providers returned surveys that included a vignette with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, and 159 providers returned surveys that included a vignette with no diagnostic label of schizophrenia. Health care providers who responded to these surveys fell into 5 main categories: primary care physicians (15.7%), psychiatrists (17.6%), psychologists (21.7%), primary care nurses (25.9%), and mental health nurses (19.1%). Most providers were female (65%), white (63%), and aged ≥ 40 years (68.7%). The mean number of years in clinical practice for each study participant was approximately 17 years. Only 22% reported having no contact with friends or family members with mental illness. The pairwise correlations among the provider stereotype, prejudice, and social distance measures were moderate. Figure 1 shows the results of the mediational analysis using path analysis, while adjusting for the demographic measures. The results indicate that there was a significant positive correlation between provider stereotypes and prejudice (β = 0.298, P < .0001), and prejudice significantly predicted social distance (β = 0.190, P = .002). As would be expected from these results, the indirect effect of stereotypes on social distance, tested using bootstrapped standard errors, was also statistically significant (β = 0.167, P = .007) (Figure 1).
DISCUSSION
The findings from this study support the hypothesized mediational model that prejudice mediates the relationship between provider stereotypes and discriminatory behavior (social distance). While the relationship between stereotypes and social distance is statistically significant, the strength of their association through prejudice is stronger (see Figure 1). The significance of this report is 2-fold.
First, our study upholds the important role of affective reactions (prejudice) in generating discriminatory behavior (social distancing) among health care providers. This finding in a sample of health care providers is similar to the findings reported by Jussim and colleagues2 in a sample of university students. These researchers2 tested 3 different models: an affective model that assumed prejudice mediates bias, a cognition model that assumed stereotypical beliefs mediated bias, and a third model that assumed both affective reactions and cognitive knowledge structures are necessary to mediate bias. In all 3 tests, they found that controlling for prejudice eliminated bias. Two of their studies also suggested that controlling for stereotypes eliminated bias.2 Findings from our study indicate that negative stereotypes could result in stronger discriminatory behaviors when mediated by prejudicial attitudes. Although negative stereotypes alone could result in discriminatory behavior, such effect is weaker when compared with the mediational role played by prejudice.
Second, this study aids in understanding potential targets for interventions to decrease stigma among health care providers. A review of literature on intervention strategies for reducing stigma in the general public showed that education and contact-based interventions were promising.21 Using a didactic approach, "education" contrasts the myths of SMI with facts to dispel stereotypes rooted in misconceptions (cognitive distortions). Using an experiential approach, "contact" (with individuals with SMI) challenges stereotypes and attempts to alter negative automatic affective responses. However, effect sizes for these interventions are quite modest in published studies.21 These studies21 were predominantly tested using study subjects drawn from college students. It is unclear if similar strategies would be effective to target stigmatizing stereotypes and discriminatory behaviors endorsed by health care providers.
We recently pilot tested both education and contact-based interventions in a sample of primary care providers at a VA facility using a randomized comparative effectiveness trial (manuscript in process). While there was some evidence from the quantitative data to suggest that education-based intervention was beneficial, the qualitative analysis suggested that contact-based intervention was perceived to be impactful. Findings from contemporary research22 in the general public suggest that using education or contact alone may not be sufficient to bring desired changes in attitudes and discriminatory behavior toward persons with mental illness. Individuals with established negative attitudes about mental illness are less likely to be influenced by education alone than individuals with generally neutral or positive attitudes.22 Studies23 also suggest that education-based strategies would be more useful if they incorporated cognitive and emotional components such as a cinematic experience rather than traditional classroom material. Such findings22,23 strengthen a hypothesis that using a combined education and contact-based intervention might be an effective strategy to target stigma in health care providers.
It is recommended that health care providers, particularly primary care providers, be more aware of the role of stigmatizing stereotypes in undermining their decision-making process. The mediational model tested in this study, and findings from contemporary literature1,2 on stigmatizing attitudes held by the general public, suggest that discriminatory behavior is quite severe when individuals hold and endorse stigmatizing stereotypes and that prejudice influences this effect. While stereotypes are cognitive knowledge structures that could be targeted effectively by educational strategies, prejudicial affective reactions could be moderated by increasing contact with high-functioning individuals with lived experience of SMI. We recommend that future researchers develop and test such combined intervention strategies for health care providers. Findings from our study will help researchers identify potential targets for these interventions. For instance, interventions could be successful if they target negative expectations and prejudice held by health care providers toward individuals with SMI. Incorporation of educational components that highlight awareness regarding automated affective reactions and their effects on clinicians’ health care decisions would also be helpful. Lastly, findings from this study are based on a relatively small sample of VA health care providers, which could limit generalizability.
Submitted: December 26, 2017; accepted March 13, 2018.
Published online: May 24, 2018.
Potential conflicts of interest: None.
Funding/support: This research was supported by grant IIR 08-086 from the VA Health Services Research and Development Service, the Central Arkansas Veterans Healthcare System, and the South Central Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center.
Role of the sponsor: The funding agencies had no role in the design and conduct of this secondary data analysis including collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of data or preparation, review, and approval of the manuscript.
Disclaimer: The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the US Department of Veterans Affairs.
REFERENCES
1. Corrigan P, Markowitz FE, Watson A, et al. An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(2):162-179. PubMed CrossRef
2. Jussim L, Nelson TE, Manis M, et al. Prejudice, stereotypes, and labeling effects: sources of bias in person perception. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68(2):228-246. CrossRef
3. Corrigan PW, Rao D. On the self-stigma of mental illness: stages, disclosure, and strategies for change. Can J Psychiatry. 2012;57(8):464-469. PubMed CrossRef
4. Druss BG, Bradford DW, Rosenheck RA, et al. Mental disorders and use of cardiovascular procedures after myocardial infarction. JAMA. 2000;283(4):506-511. PubMed CrossRef
5. Nordt C, Rossler W, Lauber C. Attitudes of mental health professionals toward people with schizophrenia and major depression. Schizophr Bull. 2005;32(4):709-714. PubMed CrossRef
6. Lauber C, Nordt C, Braunschweig C, et al. Do mental health professionals stigmatize their patients? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;113(s429):51-59. PubMed CrossRef
7. Van Dorn RA, Swanson JW, Elbogen EB, et al. A comparison of stigmatizing attitudes toward persons with schizophrenia in four stakeholder groups: perceived likelihood of violence and desire for social distance. Psychiatry. 2005;68(2):152-163. PubMed CrossRef
8. Schulze B. Stigma and mental health professionals: a review of the evidence on an intricate relationship. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007;19(2):137-155. PubMed CrossRef
9. Magliano L, Fiorillo A, De Rosa C, et al. Beliefs about schizophrenia in Italy: a comparative nationwide survey of the general public, mental health professionals, and patients’ relatives. Can J Psychiatry. 2004;49(5):322-330. PubMed CrossRef
10. Lepping P, Steinert T, Gebhardt R-P, et al. Attitudes of mental health professionals and lay-people towards involuntary admission and treatment in England and Germany—a questionnaire analysis. Eur Psychiatry. 2004;19(2):91-95. PubMed CrossRef
11. Corrigan PW, Druss BG, Perlick DA. The impact of mental illness stigma on seeking and participating in mental health care. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2014;15(2):37-70. PubMed CrossRef
12. Corrigan PW, Mittal D, Reaves CM, et al. Mental health stigma and primary health care decisions. Psychiatry Res. 2014;218(1-2):35-38. PubMed CrossRef
13. Durand-Zaleski I, Scott J, Rouillon F, et al. A first national survey of knowledge, attitudes and behaviours towards schizophrenia, bipolar disorders and autism in France. BMC Psychiatry. 2012;12(1):128. PubMed CrossRef
14. Sullivan G, Mittal D, Reaves CM, et al. Influence of schizophrenia diagnosis on providers’ practice decisions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2015;76(8):1068-1074. PubMed CrossRef
15. Mittal D, Ounpraseuth ST, Reaves C, et al. Providers’ personal and professional contact with persons with mental illness: relationship to clinical expectations. Psychiatr Serv. 2016;67(1):55-61. PubMed CrossRef
16. Mittal D, Corrigan P, Sherman MD, et al. Healthcare providers’ attitudes toward persons with schizophrenia. Psychiatr Rehabil J. 2014;37(4):297-303. PubMed CrossRef
17. Olmsted DW, Durham K. Stability of mental health attitudes: a semantic differential study. J Health Soc Behav. 1976;17(1):35. PubMed CrossRef
18. Corrigan PW, Rowan D, Green A, et al. Challenging two mental illness stigmas: personal responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophr Bull. 2002;28(2):293-309. PubMed CrossRef
19. National Opinion Research Center. General Social Survey (GSS). 1972-2006. The National Data Program for the Sciences, University of Chicago. NORC website. http://www.norc.org/Research/Projects/Pages/general-social-survey.aspx. Accessed April 27, 2018.
20. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables: User’s Guide. 6th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2010.
21. Corrigan PW, Morris SB, Michaels PJ, et al. Challenging the public stigma of mental illness: a meta-analysis of outcome studies. Psychiatr Serv. 2012;63(10):963-973. PubMed CrossRef
22. Boysen GA, Vogel DL. Education and mental health stigma: the effects of attribution, biased assimilation, and attitude polarization. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2008;27(5):447-470. CrossRef
23. Zeppegno P, Gramaglia C, Feggi A, et al. The effectiveness of a new approach using movies in the training of medical students. Perspect Med Educ. 2015;4(5):261-263. PubMed CrossRef
Author Affiliations
Enjoy free PDF downloads as part of your membership!
Save
Cite